-
46Hello darkness my old friend: preferences for darkness vary by neuroticism and co-occur with negative affectCognition and Emotion 33 (5): 885-900. 2018.ABSTRACTMetaphors frequently link negative affect with darkness and associations of this type have been established in several experimental paradigms. Given the ubiquity and strength of these associations, people who prefer dark to light may be more prone to negative emotional experiences and symptoms. A five study investigation couches these ideas in a new theoretical framework and then examines them. Across studies, 1 in 4 people preferred the perceptual concept of dark over the perceptual con…Read more
-
43The negative feedback dysregulation effect: losses of motor control in response to negative feedbackCognition and Emotion 33 (3): 536-547. 2018.ABSTRACTNegative feedback has paradoxical features to it. This form of feedback can have informational value under some circumstances, but it can also threaten the ego, potentially upsetting behaviour as a result. To investigate possible consequences of the latter type, two experiments presented positive or negative feedback within a sequence-prediction task that could not be solved. Following feedback, participants had to control their behaviours as effectively as possible in a motor control ta…Read more
-
41Counting to ten milliseconds: Low-anger, but not high-anger, individuals pause following negative evaluationsCognition and Emotion 26 (2): 261-281. 2012.The emotion of anger, when chronic, is especially problematic. Frequent and intense experiences of anger predict quite a few adverse health outcomes and are especially implicated in cardiovascular...
-
38Cognitive sources of evidence for neuroticism's link to punishment-reactivity processesCognition and Emotion 24 (5): 741-759. 2010.Theories of neuroticism emphasise its close potential link to punishment-reactivity processes, yet cognitive sources of evidence for this proposed processing basis are surprisingly scarce. The present two studies (N = 123) sought to rectify this important gap in the literature in terms of reactivity to error feedback. Study 1 found that individuals high in neuroticism were faster to switch behavioural responses following errors, whereas an opposite pattern was found among individuals low in neur…Read more
-
34Understanding Personality and Predicting Outcomes: The Utility of Cognitive-Behavioral Probes of Approach and Avoidance MotivationEmotion Review 5 (3): 303-307. 2013.Approach and avoidance motivation may represent important explanatory constructs in understanding how individuals differ. Such constructs have primarily been assessed in self-reported terms, but there are limitations to self-reports of motivation. Accordingly, the present review concentrates on the potential utility of implicit cognitive-behavioral probes of approach and avoidance motivation in modeling and understanding individual differences. The review summarizes multiple lines of research th…Read more
-
34Living large: Affect amplification in visual perception predicts emotional reactivity to events in daily lifeCognition and Emotion 27 (3): 453-464. 2013.A quick mental survey of one's friends or acquaintances reveals an important difference between them. On the one hand, there are seemingly stoic people for whom emotional events (e.g., having a pap...
-
33Cognitive-emotional dysfunction among noisy minds: Predictions from individual differences in reaction time variabilityCognition and Emotion 25 (2): 307-327. 2011.No abstract
-
32Anger as “seeing red”: Evidence for a perceptual associationCognition and Emotion 26 (8): 1445-1458. 2012.
-
32Routine cognitive errors: A trait-like predictor of individual differences in anxiety and distressCognition and Emotion 25 (2): 244-264. 2011.No abstract
-
31Are self-deceivers enhancing positive affect or denying negative affect? Toward an understanding of implicit affective processesCognition and Emotion 23 (1): 152-180. 2009.Self-deception is an important construct in social, personality, and clinical literatures. Although historical and clinical views of self-deception have regarded it as defensive in nature and operation, modern views of this individual difference variable instead highlight its apparent benefits to subjective mental health. The present four studies reinforce the latter view by showing that self-deception predicts positive priming effects, but not negative priming effects, in reaction time tasks se…Read more
-
28Running from William James' Bear: A Review of Preattentive Mechanisms and their Contributions to Emotional Experience (review)Cognition and Emotion 12 (5): 667-696. 1998.
-
28The Reactive and Prospective Functions of Mood: Its Role in Linking Daily Experiences and Cognitive Well-beingCognition and Emotion 14 (2): 145-176. 2000.How do daily life events impact the affective and cognitive components of well-being (WB)? Results from cross-sectional (Ns = 129 and 64) and longitudinal (N = 129) studies suggest that there is a parsimonious answer to this question. Life events are closely related to mood states, mood states are closely related to cognitive WB, and mood states appear to serve as the nexus through which changing life circumstances affect cognitive WB. These results are consistent with a mood-mediation model, wh…Read more
-
27Clear heads are cool heads: Emotional clarity and the down-regulation of antisocial affectCognition and Emotion 22 (2): 308-326. 2008.
-
26Seven sins in the study of unconscious affectIn Lisa Feldman Barrett, Paula M. Niedenthal & Piotr Winkielman (eds.), Emotion and Consciousness, Guilford Press. pp. 384-408. 2005.
-
22Losing control, literally: Relations between anger control, trait anger, and motor controlCognition and Emotion 27 (6): 995-1012. 2013.
-
University of Southern CaliforniaRegular Faculty
Los Angeles, California, United States of America