•  13
    The evolution of imagination and the adaptive value of imaginary worlds
    with Thomas Hills
    Behavioral and Brain Sciences 45. 2022.
    Characterizing the cultural evolution of imaginary worlds as a hedonic but non-adaptive exaptation from evolved exploratory tendencies, Dubourg and Baumard defend too narrow a conception of the adaptive evolution of imaginary worlds. Imagination and its imaginary worlds are ancient and adaptive, allowing deliberation over actions, consequences, and futures worth aspiring to, often engendering the world we see around us.
  •  13
    The evolution of imagination and the adaptive value of imaginary worlds
    with Thomas Hills
    Behavioral and Brain Sciences 45. 2022.
    Characterizing the cultural evolution of imaginary worlds as a hedonic but non-adaptive exaptation from evolved exploratory tendencies, Dubourg and Baumard defend too narrow a conception of the adaptive evolution of imaginary worlds. Imagination and its imaginary worlds are ancient and adaptive, allowing deliberation over actions, consequences, and futures worth aspiring to, often engendering the world we see around us.
  •  10
    Pragmatic interpretation and the production of ideographic codes
    with Leda Berio, Berke Can, Katharina Helming, and Giulia Palazzolo
    Behavioral and Brain Sciences 46. 2023.
    We argue that the problem of ideographic codes stems from neither learnability nor standardization, but from a general issue of pragmatic interpretation. As ideographic codes increase in expressive power, in order to reduce ambiguity, they must become more detailed – such that production becomes more cumbersome, and requires greater artistry on the part of users, limiting their capacity for growth.
  •  8
    Rethinking how children individuate objects: spatial indexicals in early development
    with Frauke Hildebrandt and Ramiro Glauer
    Synthese 202 (3): 1-25. 2023.
    The current understanding of cognitive development rests on the premise that infants can individuate objects early on. However, the so-called object-first account faces severe difficulties explaining extant empirical findings in object individuation tasks while alternative, more parsimonious explanations are available. In this paper, we assume that children start as feature-thinkers without being able to individuate objects and show how this ability can be learned by thinkers who do not already …Read more
  •  2
    I'm a Believer (review)
    Times Literary Supplement 20. 2015.
  •  2
    A prevailing view is that while human communication has an ‘ostensive-inferential’ or ‘Gricean’ intentional structure, animal communication does not. This would make the psychological states that support human and animal forms of communication fundamentally different. Against this view, I argue that there are grounds to expect ostensive communication in non-human clades. This is because it is sufficient for ostensive communication that one intentionally address one’s utterance to one’s intended …Read more
  •  1
    Two-year-olds use adults’ but not peers’ points
    with Gregor Kachel and Michael Tomasello
    Developmental Science 1-9. 2018.
    In the current study, 24- to 27-month-old children (N = 37) used pointing gestures in a cooperative object choice task with either peer or adult partners. When indicating the location of a hidden toy, children pointed equally accurately for adult and peer partners but more often for adult partners. When choosing from one of three hiding places, children used adults’ pointing to find a hidden toy significantly more often than they used peers’. In interaction with peers, children’s choice behavior…Read more
  •  1
    Great apes search for longer following humans’ ostensive signals, but do not then follow their gaze.
    with Fumihiro Kano, Chris Krupenye, Satoshi Hirata, Masaki Tomongaga, and Josep Call
    Animal Cognition 21 (5): 715-728. 2018.
    The previous studies have shown that human infants and domestic dogs follow the gaze of a human agent only when the agent has addressed them ostensively—e.g., by making eye contact, or calling their name. This evidence is interpreted as showing that they expect ostensive signals to precede referential information. The present study tested chimpanzees, one of the closest relatives to humans, in a series of eye-tracking experiments using an experimental design adapted from these previous studies. …Read more
  • Evolutionary Pragmatics (edited book)
    Oxford University Press. forthcoming.
  • The Origins of European Dissent
    Utopian Studies 7 (1): 134-135. 1996.
  • Toddlers prefer adults as informants: Two- and three-year- olds’ use of and attention to pointing gestures from peer and adult partners
    with Gregor Kachel, Robert Hepach, and Michael Tomasello
    Child Development (1): 1-18. 2021.
    Two‐ and 3‐year‐old children (N = 96) were tested in an object‐choice task with video presentations of peer and adult partners. An immersive, semi‐interactive procedure enabled both the close matching of adult and peer conditions and the combination of participants’ choice behavior with looking time measures. Children were more likely to use information provided by adults. As the effect was more pronounced in the younger age‐group, the observed bias may fade during toddlerhood. As there were no …Read more