•  2
    Epistola de Tolerantia, A Letter on Toleration
    with Raymond Klibansky and J. W. Gough
    Tijdschrift Voor Filosofie 31 (3): 591-592. 1969.
  •  13
    This work has been selected by scholars as being culturally important, and is part of the knowledge base of civilization as we know it. This work was reproduced from the original artifact, and remains as true to the original work as possible. Therefore, you will see the original copyright references, library stamps (as most of these works have been housed in our most important libraries around the world), and other notations in the work. This work is in the public domain in the United States of …Read more
  •  14
    This work has been selected by scholars as being culturally important, and is part of the knowledge base of civilization as we know it. This work was reproduced from the original artifact, and remains as true to the original work as possible. Therefore, you will see the original copyright references, library stamps (as most of these works have been housed in our most important libraries around the world), and other notations in the work.This work is in the public domain in the United States of A…Read more
  •  8
    The Philosophical Works of John Locke
    with James Augustus St John
    George Bell & Sons. 1898.
  •  19
    Electromyography and lipreading in the detection of verbal rehearsal
    with Mickey Ginsburg
    Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 5 (3): 246-248. 1975.
  •  30
    Deaf children's phonetic, visual, and dactylic coding in a grapheme recall task
    with Virginia L. Locke
    Journal of Experimental Psychology 89 (1): 142. 1971.
  •  32
    ... a thinking intelligent Being, that has reason and reflection, and can consider itself as itself, the same thinking thing in different times and places; which it only does by the consciousness, which is inseparable from thinking ... [Essay II, xxvii, '9].
  •  36
    The need for psychological needs: A role for social capital
    with Catherine M. Flanagan
    Behavioral and Brain Sciences 36 (5): 495-496. 2013.
    Van de Vliert embraces a model of human needs, underplaying a model whereby individuals, motivated by psychological needs, develop coping strategies that help them meet their personal goals and collectively exert an influence on social and economic systems. Undesirable climates may inflate the value of financial capital, but they also boost the value of social capital
  •  30
    Dancing with humans: Interaction as unintended consequence
    Behavioral and Brain Sciences 25 (5): 632-633. 2002.
    Parallels to Shanker & King's (S&K's) proposal for a model of language teaching that values dyadic interaction have long existed in language development, for the neotenous human infant requires care, which is inherently interactive. Interaction with talking caregivers facilitates language learning. The “new” paradigm thus has a decidedly familiar look. It would be surprising if some other paradigm worked better in animals that have no evolutionary linguistic history.
  •  34
    Trickle-up phonetics: A vocal role for the infant
    Behavioral and Brain Sciences 27 (4): 516-516. 2004.
    Falk claims that human language took a step forward when infants lost their ability to cling and were placed on the ground, increasing their fears, which mothers assuaged prosodically. This claim, which is unsupported by anthropological and psychological evidence, would have done little for the syllabic and segmental structure of language, and ignores infants' own contribution to the process.
  •  99
    Language and life history: A new perspective on the development and evolution of human language
    with Barry Bogin
    Behavioral and Brain Sciences 29 (3): 259-280. 2006.
    It has long been claimed that Homo sapiens is the only species that has language, but only recently has it been recognized that humans also have an unusual pattern of growth and development. Social mammals have two stages of pre-adult development: infancy and juvenility. Humans have two additional prolonged and pronounced life history stages: childhood, an interval of four years extending between infancy and the juvenile period that follows, and adolescence, a stage of about eight years that str…Read more
  •  45
    The trait of human language: Lessons from the canal boat children of England
    Biology and Philosophy 23 (3): 347-361. 2008.
    To fully understand human language, an evolved trait that develops in the young without formal instruction, it must be possible to observe language that has not been influenced by instruction. But in modern societies, much of the language that is used, and most of the language that is measured, is confounded by literacy and academic training. This diverts empirical attention from natural habits of speech, causing theorists to miss critical features of linguistic practice. To dramatize this point…Read more
  •  33
    Parental selection of vocal behavior
    Human Nature 17 (2): 155-168. 2006.
    Although all natural languages are spoken, there is no accepted account of the evolution of a skill prerequisite to language—control of the movements of speech. If selection applied at sexual maturity, individuals achieving some command of articulate vocal behavior in previous stages would have enjoyed unusual advantages in adulthood. I offer a parental selection hypothesis, according to which hominin parents apportioned care, in part, on the basis of their infants’ vocal behavior. Specifically,…Read more
  •  45
    Vocal innovation
    Behavioral and Brain Sciences 30 (4): 415-416. 2007.
    An important form of innovation involves use of the voice in a new way, usually to solve some environmental problem. Vocal innovation occurs in humans and other animals, including chimpanzees. The framework outlined in the target article, appropriately modified, may permit new perspectives on the use of others as tools, especially by infants, and the evolution of speech and language