• Reply to Pincock
    The Bertrand Russell Society Quarterly 125. 2005.
  •  37
    Coming of age in Olduvai and the Zaire rain forest
    Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18 (1): 196-197. 1995.
    ProbablyHomo habilisis two species not one; similarly for Pan troglodytes. Although amenable to training, in naturePan paniscusmay be a “specialized insular dwarf.” Language is uniquely human, but symbolic behavior and intelligence are widespread among animals with little respect for phylogenetic closeness toHomo sapiens.
  • An Invitation to Cognitive Science
    Inquiry: An Interdisciplinary Journal of Philosophy 36 (n/a): 179. 1993.
  •  66
    Book reviews (review)
    with Eric A. Weiss, Judith Felson Duchan, Mallory Selfridge, Eric Dietrich, Peter A. Facione, Timothy Joseph Day, Johan M. Lammens, Andrew Feenberg, Deborah G. Johnson, Daniel S. Levine, and Ted A. Warfield
    Minds and Machines 5 (1): 109-155. 1995.
  •  155
    Linguistic analysis and existentialism
    Philosophy and Phenomenological Research 32 (1): 47-56. 1971.
  •  39
    The “Many Pun” Argument
    Southern Journal of Philosophy 1 (1): 36-39. 1963.
  •  51
    Insulting
    Philosophia 8 (4): 549-571. 1979.
  •  50
    Descartes: The Smear and Related Misconstruals
    Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour 41 (4): 365-376. 2011.
    In part because he is known through his Meditations, a short pamphlet he wrote, rightly in fear, to conciliate (unsuccessfully) with the church, and because his rationalism is misconstrued when interpreted empirically, Descartes is subject to a variety of misunderstandings. It does not help that he is dogged by a canard invented in the late 1600s and revived by the animal rights movement, a canard that was designed to denigrate the then burgeoning mechanistic new science, discovered cruelly cutt…Read more
  •  19
    Aesthetic emotion
    Southern Journal of Philosophy 6 (4): 215-223. 1968.
  •  3
    The First Social Psychologist
    Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour 38 (4): 489-493. 2008.
    Because both the individual and the state call for simultaneous investigation in social psychology, one could argue that in some sense Serge Moscovici, with the publication of Psychoanalysis, put forward the first systematic large scale empirical study of cognitive representations in, as it happily proved, various blocs of French understandings of psychoanalysis, admitting, characteristically, doubts about the individual, bloc, and the state representations
  •  4
    Structuralism
    Philosophy and Phenomenological Research 39 (4): 598-599. 1979.
  • Alan Turing draws a firm line between the mental and the physical, between the cognitive and physical sciences. For Turing, following a tradition that went back to D=Arcy Thompson, if not Geoffroy and Lucretius, throws talk of function, intentionality, and final causes from biology as a physical science. He likens Amother nature@ to the earnest A. I. scientist, who may send to school disparate versions of the Achild machine,@ eventually hoping for a test-passer but knowing that the vagaries of h…Read more
  •  9
    In his short life, Alan Turing (1912-1954) made foundational contributions to philosophy, mathematics, biology, artificial intelligence, and computer science. He, as much as anyone, invented the digital electronic computer. From September, 1939 much of his work on computation was war-driven and brutally practical. He developed high speed computing devices needed to decipher German Enigma Machine messages to and from U-boats, countering the most serious threat by far to Britain's survival during …Read more
  •  11
    Logic as Grammar (review)
    Review of Metaphysics 39 (4): 772-773. 1986.
    This is an excellent book for philosophers, and others concerned with natural language and cognition, who have not kept up with post-Aspects work in syntax, in particular with the Extended Standard Theory work on government and binding that relates to anaphora and quantification. It is a direct challenge to those who think that there must be a reasonably clearcut semantic level of description for sentences in natural language, one which is crucial for explaining how we learn, understand, and use…Read more
  •  19
    The Nature of Psychological Explanation (review)
    Review of Metaphysics 40 (1): 109-110. 1986.
    This spare book amply maintains the distinction of the Bradford Book series. In chapter 1 Cummins argues that the familiar deductive-nomological notion of scientific explanation only covers transitional theories and fails to give an account of explanation through property or system analysis that is pervasive in both the physical and psychological sciences. This inadequacy of the D-N view is supposed particularly injurious in the unrobust and infant science of psychology. Explanation through anal…Read more
  •  107
    Instinctive incest avoidance: A paradigm case for evolutionary psychology evaporates
    Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour 36 (4). 2006.
    Westermarck proposed that humans have an incest avoidance instinct, triggered by frequent intimate contact with family members during the first several years of life. Westermarck reasons that familial incest will tend to produce less fit offspring, those humans without instinctive incest avoidance would hence have tended to die off and those with the avoidance instinct would have produced more viable offspring, and hence familial incest would be, as indeed it is, universally and instinctively av…Read more
  •  37
    Faculty before folk
    Behavioral and Brain Sciences 21 (4): 579-580. 1998.
    Pace Atran, (1) folk physics, (2) folk biology, and (3) folk psychology rest on informationally encapsulated modules that emerge before language: a gifted austic person who can see objects and animals perfectly well can nonetheless be incommunicatively mind blind.