•  2545
    The mnemonic basis of subjective experience
    with Hakwan Lau, Matthias Michel, and Stephen Fleming
    Nature Reviews Psychology. 2022.
    Conscious experiences involve subjective qualities, such as colours, sounds, smells and emotions. In this Perspective, we argue that these subjective qualities can be understood in terms of their similarity to other experiences. This account highlights the role of memory in conscious experience, even for simple percepts. How an experience feels depends on implicit memory of the relationships between different perceptual representations within the brain. With more complex experiences such as emot…Read more
  •  1589
    Understanding the Higher-Order Approach to Consciousness
    Trends in Cognitive Sciences 23 (9): 754-768. 2019.
    Critics have often misunderstood the higher-order theory (HOT) of consciousness. Here we clarify its position on several issues, and distinguish it from other views such as the global The higher-order theory (HOT) of consciousness has often been misunderstood by critics. Here we clarify its position on several issues, and distinguish it from other views such as the global workspace theory (GWT) and early sensory models (e.g. first-order local recurrency theories). For example, HOT has been criti…Read more
  •  682
    Higher-Order Memory Schema and Conscious Experience
    Cognitive Neuropsychology 37 (3-4): 213-215. 2020.
    In the interesting and thought-provoking article Grazziano and colleagues argue for their Attention Schema Theory (AST) of consciousness. They present AST as a unification of Global Workspace Theory (GWT), Illusionism, and the Higher-Order Thought (HOT) theory. We argue it is a mistake to equate 'subjective experience,' ad related terms, with dualism. They simply denote experience. Also, as presented, AST does not accurately capture the essence of HOT for two reasons. HOT is presented as a versi…Read more
  •  90
    The slippery slope of fear
    Trends in Cognitive Sciences 17 (4): 155-156. 2013.
    'Fear' is used scientifically in two ways, which causes confusion: it refers to conscious feelings and to behavioral and physiological responses. Restricting the use of 'fear' to denote feelings and using 'threat-induced defensive reactions' for the responses would help avoid misunderstandings about the brain mechanisms involved
  •  60
    The “happiness agenda” is a worldwide movement that claims that happiness is the highest good, happiness can be measured, and public policy should promote happiness. Against Happiness is a thorough and powerful critique of this program, revealing the flaws of its concept of happiness and advocating a renewed focus on equality and justice. Written by an interdisciplinary team of authors, this book provides both theoretical and empirical analysis of the limitations of the happiness agenda. The aut…Read more
  •  38
    A little history goes a long way toward understanding why we study consciousness the way we do today
    Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 1. 2020.
    Consciousness is currently a thriving area of research in psychology and neuroscience. While this is often attributed to events that took place in the early 1990s, consciousness studies today are a continuation of research that started in the late 19th century and that continued throughout the 20th century. From the beginning, the effort built on studies of animals to reveal basic principles of brain organization and function, and of human patients to gain clues about consciousness itself. Parti…Read more
  •  29
    Emotional circuits and computational neuroscience
    with Jean-Marc Fellous and Jorge L. Armony
    In M. Arbib (ed.), The Handbook of Brain Theory and Neural Networks, Mit Press. pp. 2. 2002.
  •  23
    The essence of who we are depends on our brains. They enable us to think, to feel joy and sorrow, communicate through speech, reflect on the moments of our lives, and to anticipate, plan for, and worry about our imagined futures. Although some of our abilities are comparatively new, key features of our behavior have deep roots that can be traced to the beginning of life. By following the story of behavior, step-by-step, over its roughly four-billion-year trajectory, we come to understand both ho…Read more
  •  20
    The Contribution of the Amygdala to Aversive and Appetitive Pavlovian Processes
    with Justin M. Moscarello
    Emotion Review 5 (3): 248-253. 2013.
    Pavlovian cues predict the occurrence of motivationally salient outcomes, thus serving as an important trigger of approach and avoidance behavior. The amygdala is a key substrate of Pavlovian conditioning, and the nature of its contribution varies by the motivational valence of unconditioned stimuli. The literature on aversive Pavlovian learning supports a serial-processing model of amygdalar function, while appetitive studies suggest that Pavlovian associations are processed through parallel ci…Read more
  •  18
    Deep history and beyond: a reply to commentators
    Philosophical Psychology 36 (4): 756-766. 2023.
    The commentaries by Ren, de Carvalho, Gabriel, Reber and Baluška raise interesting and timely questions about the views I expressed in The Deep History of Ourselves. I begin my response with an Overview of my perspective, and how it has changed in the three years since publication. This is important since some of the commentators’ concerns may be assuaged by some of these points. Other specific issues raised by each commentator are addressed separately. I greatly appreciate the time and effort t…Read more
  •  17
    A stereotaxic map of brainstem areas critical for locomotor responses in a novel environment
    with Robert Thompson
    Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 6 (3): 327-328. 1975.
  •  16
    Emotional plasticity
    with Glenn E. Schafe
    In J. Wixted & H. Pashler (eds.), Stevens' Handbook of Experimental Psychology, Wiley. 2002.
  •  15
    Brightness discrimination loss after lesions of the corpus striatum in the white rat
    with Robert Thompson and Holly Chetta
    Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 3 (4): 293-295. 1974.
  •  14
    Comment: What’s Basic About the Brain Mechanisms of Emotion?
    Emotion Review 6 (4): 318-320. 2014.
    While it is common to think that neuroscientists are proponents of basic emotions theory, this is not necessarily the case. My ideas, for example are more aligned with cognitive than basic emotions theories.
  •  12
    Longlisted for the PEN/E.O. Wilson Literary Science Writing Award A leading neuroscientist offers a history of the evolution of the brain from unicellular organisms to the complexity of animals and human beings today Renowned neuroscientist Joseph LeDoux digs into the natural history of life on earth to provide a new perspective on the similarities between us and our ancestors in deep time. This page-turning survey of the whole of terrestrial evolution sheds new light on how nervous systems evol…Read more
  •  5
    The psychology of memory
    with Daniel L. Schacter and W. Hirst
    In David A. Oakley (ed.), Mind and Brain, Methuen. pp. 189. 1986.
  •  4
    A psychologist's reply
    with D. L. Schacter and W. Hirst
    In David A. Oakley (ed.), Mind and Brain, Methuen. 1986.
  •  3
    Learning and memory: Basic mechanisms
    with J. M. Beggs, T. H. Brown, J. H. Byrne, T. Crow, K. LeBar, and R. F. Thompson
    In M. J. Zigmond & F. E. Bloom (eds.), Fundamental Neuroscience, . 1999.
  • Fundamental neuroscience
    with J. M. Beggs, T. H. Brown, J. H. Byrne, T. Crow, K. LeBar, and R. F. Thompson
    In M. J. Zigmond & F. E. Bloom (eds.), Fundamental Neuroscience, . 1999.
  • Emotions: How I've Looked for Them in the Brain
    In Robert J. Russell (ed.), Neuroscience and the person: scientific perspectives on divine action, Center For Theology and the Natural Sciences. pp. 41--56. 2002.
  • The Self - Ancient and Modern
    with Timothy J. Reiss, Matthew S. Santirocco, Phillip Mitsis, and Eva Cantarella
    New York University Press. 2000.