•  330
    Visual sensing without seeing
    Psychological Science 15 27-32. 2004.
    It has often been assumed that when we use vision to become aware of an object or event in our surroundings, this must be accompanied by a corresponding visual experience (i.e., seeing). The studies reported here show that this assumption is incorrect. When observers view a sequence of displays alternating between an image of a scene and the same image changed in some way, they often feel (or sense) the change even though they have no visual experience of it. The subjective difference between se…Read more
  •  248
    The possibility of a science of magic
    with Gustav Kuhn
    Frontiers in Psychology 6 1576. 2015.
    The past few years have seen a resurgence of interest in the scientific study of magic. Despite being only a few years old, this “new wave” has already resulted in a host of interesting studies, often using methods that are both powerful and original. These developments have largely borne out our earlier hopes (Kuhn et al., 2008) that new opportunities were available for scientific studies based on the use of magic. And it would seem that much more can still be done along these lines. But in add…Read more
  •  735
    The dynamic representation of scenes
    Visual Cognition 7 (1/2/3): 17-42. 2000.
    One of the more powerful impressions created by vision is that of a coherent, richly-detailed world where everything is present simultaneously. Indeed, this impression is so compelling that we tend to ascribe these properties not only to the external world, but to our internal representations as well. But results from several recent experiments argue against this latter ascription. For example, changes in images of real-world scenes often go unnoticed when made during a saccade, flicker, blink, …Read more
  •  258
    This chapter presents an overview of several recent developments in vision science, and outlines some of their implications for the management of visual attention in graphic displays. These include ways of sending attention to the right item at the right time, techniques to improve attentional efficiency, and possibilities for offloading some of the processing typically done by attention onto nonattentional mechanisms. In addition it is argued that such techniques not only allow more effective u…Read more
  •  238
    The influence of cast shadows on visual search
    with Patrick Cavanagh
    Perception 33 1339-1358. 2004.
    We show that cast shadows can have a significant influence on the speed of visual search. In particular, we find that search based on the shape of a region is affected when the region is darker than the background and corresponds to a shadow formed by lighting from above. Results support the proposal that an early-level system rapidly identifies regions as shadows and then discounts them, making their shapes more difficult to access. Several constraints used by this system are mapped out, includ…Read more
  •  470
    The perception of correlation in scatterplots
    with Gideon Baldridge
    Computer Graphics Forum 29 1203-1210. 2010.
    We present a rigorous way to evaluate the visual perception of correlation in scatterplots, based on classical psychophysical methods originally developed for simple properties such as brightness. Although scatterplots are graphically complex, the quantity they convey is relatively simple. As such, it may be possible to assess the perception of correlation in a similar way. Scatterplots were each of 5.0  extent, containing 100 points with a bivariate normal distribution. Means were 0.5 of the r…Read more
  •  207
    The Modeling and Control of Visual Perception
    In Wayne D. Gray (ed.), Integrated Models of Cognitive Systems, Oxford University Press. pp. 132-148. 2007.
    Recent developments in vision science have resulted in several major changes in our understanding of human visual perception. For example, attention no longer appears necessary for "visual intelligence"--a large amount of sophisticated processing can be done without it. Scene perception no longer appears to involve static, general-purpose descriptions, but instead may involve dynamic representations whose content depends on the individual and the task. And vision itself no longer appears to be l…Read more
  •  190
    The Rapid Recovery of Three-Dimensional Structure from Line Drawings
    Dissertation, University of British Columbia. 1992.
    A computational theory is developed that explains how line drawings of polyhedral objects can be interpreted rapidly and in parallel at early levels of human vision. The key idea is that a time-limited process can correctly recover much of the three-dimensional structure of these objects when split into concurrent streams, each concerned with a single aspect of scene structure.
  •  718
    Towards a science of magic
    with Gustav Kuhn and Alym A. Amlani
    Trends in Cognitive Sciences 12 (9): 349-354. 2008.
    It is argued here that cognitive science currently neglects an important source of insight into the human mind: the effects created by magicians. Over the centuries, magicians have learned how to perform acts that are perceived as defying the laws of nature, and that induce a strong sense of wonder. This article argues that the time has come to examine the scientific bases behind such phenomena, and to create a science of magic linked to relevant areas of cognitive science. Concrete examples ar…Read more
  •  195
    Sensitivity to three-dimensional orientation in visual search
    with James T. Enns
    Psychological Science 1 (5): 323-326. 1990.
    Previous theories of early vision have assumed that visual search is based on simple two-dimensional aspects of an image, such as the orientation of edges and lines. It is shown here that search can also be based on three-dimensional orientation of objects in the corresponding scene, provided that these objects are simple convex blocks. Direct comparison shows that image-based and scene-based orientation are similar in their ability to facilitate search. These findings support the hypothesis tha…Read more
  •  169
    Rapid resumption of interrupted visual search: New insights on the interaction between memory and vision
    with Alejandro Lleras and James T. Enns
    Psychological Science 16 (9): 684-688. 2005.
    A modified visual search task demonstrates that humans are very good at resuming a search after it has been momentarily interrupted. This is shown by exceptionally rapid response time to a display that reappears after a brief interruption, even when an entirely different visual display is seen during the interruption and two different visual searches are performed simultaneously. This rapid resumption depends on the stability of the visual scene and is not due to display or response anticipatio…Read more
  •  267
    Scene Perception
    In A. E. Kazdin (ed.), Encyclopedia of Psychology, Oxford University Press. pp. 151-155. 2000.
    Scene Perception is the visual perception of an environment as viewed by an observer at any given time. It includes not only the perception of individual objects, but also such things as their relative locations, and expectations about what other kinds of objects might be encountered. Given that scene perception is so effortless for most observers, it might be thought of as something easy to understand. However, the amount of effort required by a process often bears little relation to its under…Read more
  •  118
    The analysis of resource-limited vision systems
    with Greg Provan
    Proceedings of the Thirteenth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society 1 311-316. 1991.
    This paper explores the ways in which resource limitations influence the nature of perceptual and cognitive processes. A framework is developed that allows early visual processing to be analyzed in terms of these limitations. In this approach, there is no one ``best'' system for any visual process. Rather, a spectrum of systems exists, differing in the particular trade-offs made between performance and resource requirements.
  •  318
    Seeing, sensing, and scrutinizing
    Vision Research 40 1469-1487. 2000.
    Large changes in a scene often become difficult to notice if made during an eye movement, image flicker, movie cut, or other such disturbance. It is argued here that this _change blindness_ can serve as a useful tool to explore various aspects of vision. This argument centers around the proposal that focused attention is needed for the explicit perception of change. Given this, the study of change perception can provide a useful way to determine the nature of visual attention, and to cast new li…Read more
  •  301
    Preemption effects in visual search: Evidence for low-level grouping
    with James T. Enns
    Psychological Review 102 (1): 101-130. 1995.
    Experiments are presented showing that visual search for Mueller-Lyer (ML) stimuli is based on complete configurations, rather than component segments. Segments easily detected in isolation were difficult to detect when embedded in a configuration, indicating preemption by low-level groups. This preemption—which caused stimulus components to become inaccessible to rapid search—was an all-or-nothing effect, and so could serve as a powerful test of grouping. It is shown that these effects are unli…Read more
  •  528
    On the failure to detect changes in scenes across brief interruptions
    with Kevin J. O'Regan and James J. Clark
    Visual Cognition 7 (1/2/3): 127-145. 2000.
    When brief blank fields are placed between alternating displays of an original and a modified scene, a striking failure of perception is induced: the changes become extremely difficult to notice, even when they are large, presented repeatedly, and the observer expects them to occur (Rensink, O'Regan, & Clark, 1997). To determine the mechanisms behind this induced "change blindness", four experiments examine its dependence on initial preview and on the nature of the interruptions used. Results su…Read more
  •  334
    One of the more compelling beliefs about vision is that it is based on representations that are coherent and complete, with everything in the visual field described in great detail. However, changes made during a visual disturbance are found to be difficult to see, arguing against the idea that our brains contain a detailed, picture-like representation of the scene. Instead, it is argued here that a more dynamic, "just-in-time" representation is involved, one with deep similarities to the way th…Read more
  •  269
    On the applications of change blindness
    Psychologia 51 100-106. 2008.
    An overview is presented of the ways that change blindness has been applied to the study of various issues in perception and cognition. Topics include mechanisms of change perception, allocation of attention, nonconscious perception, and cognitive beliefs. Recent work using change blindness to investigate these topics is surveyed, along with a brief discussion of some of the ways that these approaches may further develop over the next few years
  •  254
    Influence of scene-based properties on visual search
    with James T. Enns
    Science 247 721-723. 1990.
    The task of visual search is to determine as rapidly as possible whether a target item is present or absent in a display. Rapidly detected items are thought to contain features that correspond to primitive elements in the human visual system. In previous theories, it has been assumed that visual search is based on simple two-dimensional features in the image. However, visual search also has access to another level of representation, one that describes properties in the corresponding three-dimens…Read more
  •  156
    On the Visual Discrimination of Self-Similar Random Textures
    Dissertation, University of British Columbia. 1986.
    This work investigates the ability of the human visual system to discriminate self-similar Gaussian random textures. The power spectra of such textures are similar to themselves when rescaled by some factor h > 1. As such, these textures provide a natural domain for testing the hypothesis that texture perception is based on a set of spatial-frequency channels characterized by filters of similar shape.
  •  293
    Limits to the usability of iconic memory
    Frontiers in Psychology 5. 2014.
    Human vision briefly retains a trace of a stimulus after it disappears. This trace—iconic memory—is often believed to be a surrogate for the original stimulus, a representational structure that can be used as if the original stimulus were still present. To investigate its nature, a flicker-search paradigm was developed that relied upon a full scan (rather than partial report) of its contents. Results show that for visual search it can indeed act as a surrogate, with little cost for alternating b…Read more
  •  79
    Identification of highlights in early vision
    Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science 35 1623. 1994.
    Purpose. To determine whether highlights are rapidly identified at early levels of vision. Methods. Visual search experiments were carried out using simple black and white figures corresponding to shiny objects lit from various directions. These included, for example, depictions of cylinders with highlights positioned at various heights (see figure). Targets and distractors differed only in the arrangement of their constituent regions, allowing them to be distinguished by the position of the hi…Read more
  •  138
    Ideal observers, real observers, and the return of Elvis
    In David C. Knill & Whitman Richards (eds.), Perception as Bayesian Inference, Cambridge University Press. pp. 451-455. 1996.
    Knill, Kersten, & Mamassian (Chapter 6) provide an interesting discussion of how the Bayesian formulation can be used to help investigate human vision. In their view, computational theories can be based on an ideal observer that uses Bayesian inference to make optimal use of available information. Four factors are important here: the image information used, the output structures estimated, the priors assumed (i.e., knowledge about the structure of the world), and the likelihood function used (i.…Read more
  •  107
    Four Futures and a History
    The Encyclopedia of Human-Computer Interaction, 2nd Ed.; 35. Data Visualization for Human Perception. 2011.
    Stephen Few provides a nice overview of the reasons why we should design data visualizations to be effective, and why it’s important to understand human perception when doing so. In fact, he’s done this so well that I can’t add much to his arguments. But I can, however, push the basic message a bit further, out into the times before and after those he discusses. Out into areas that are not as well known, or not really developed, where new opportunities and new dangers may lie…
  •  214
    Four-sight in hindsight: The existence of magical numbers in vision
    Behavioral and Brain Sciences 24 (1): 141-142. 2001.
    The capacity of visual attention/STM can be determined by change-detection experiments. Detecting the presence of change leads to an estimate of 4 items, while detecting the absence of change leads to an estimate of 1 item. Thus, there are two magical numbers in vision: 4 and 1. The underlying limits, however, are not necessarily those of central STM.
  •  535
    Change Blindness
    In Laurent Itti, Geraint Rees & John K. Tsotsos (eds.), Neurobiology of Attention, Academic Press. pp. 76--81. 2005.
    Large changes that occur in clear view of an observer can become difficult to notice if made during an eye movement, blink, or other such disturbance. This change blindness is consistent with the proposal that focused visual attention is necessary to see change, with a change becoming difficult to notice whenever conditions prevent attention from being automatically drawn to it. It is shown here how the phenomenon of change blindness can provide new results on the nature of visual attention, inc…Read more
  •  438
    Change blindness: Implications for the nature of visual attention
    In L. Harris & M. Jenkin (eds.), Vision and Attention, Academic Press. pp. 16-20. 2001.
    In the not-too-distant past, vision was often said to involve three levels of processing: a low level concerned with descriptions of the geometric and photometric properties of the image, a high level concerned with abstract knowledge of the physical and semantic properties of the world, and a middle level concerned with anything not handled by the other two. The negative definition of mid-level vision contained in this description reflected a rather large gap in our understanding of visual proc…Read more
  •  79
    Attentional processing of geometric figures
    Perception 28 (suppl.). 1999.
    Focused attention is needed to perceive change (Rensink et al., 1997; Psychological Science, 8: 368-373) . But how much attentional processing is given to an item? And does this depend on the nature of the task? To answer these questions, "flicker" displays were created, where an original and a modified image continually alternated, with brief blanks between them. Each image was an array of simple figures, half being horizontal and the other half vertical. In half the trials, one of the items ch…Read more
  •  1597
    Change Detection
    Annual Review of Psychology 53 (1): 245-277. 2002.
    Five aspects of visual change detection are reviewed. The first concerns the concept of change itself, in particular the ways it differs from the related notions of motion and difference. The second involves the various methodological approaches that have been developed to study change detection; it is shown that under a variety of conditions observers are often unable to see large changes directly in their field of view. Next, it is argued that this “change blindness” indicates that focused att…Read more