•  315
    Visual features as carriers of abstract quantitative information
    Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 8 (151): 1793-1820. 2022.
    Four experiments investigated the extent to which abstract quantitative information can be conveyed by basic visual features. This was done by asking observers to estimate and discriminate Pearson correlation in graphical representations where the first data dimension of each element was encoded by its horizontal position, and the second by the value of one of its visual features; perceiving correlation then requires combining the information in the two encodings via a common abstract representa…Read more
  •  46
    Induced failures of visual awareness
    Journal of Vision 2 (3). 2003.
    Research over the past half century has produced extensive evidence that observers cannot report or retain all of the details of their visual world from one moment to the next. During the past decade, a new set of studies has illustrated just how pervasive these limits are. For example, early evidence for the failure to detect changes to simple dot patterns (Phillips, 1974) and arrays of letters (Pashler, 1988) generalizes to more naturalistic displays such as photographs and motion pictures (e.…Read more
  •  378
    A set of visual search experiments tested the proposal that focused attention is needed to detect change. Displays were arrays of rectangles, with the target being the item that continually changed its orientation or contrast polarity. Five aspects of performance were examined: linearity of response, processing time, capacity, selectivity, and memory trace. Detection of change was found to be a self-terminating process requiring a time that increased linearly with the number of items in the disp…Read more
  •  177
    Seeing Seeing
    PSYCHE: An Interdisciplinary Journal of Research On Consciousness 16 (1): 68-78. 2010.
    This paper discusses several key issues concerning consciousness and human vision. A brief overview is presented of recent developments in this area, including issues that have been resolved and issues that remain unsettled. Based on this, three Hilbert questions are proposed. These involve three related sets of issues: the kinds of visual experience that exist, the kinds of visual attention that exist, and the ways that these relate to each other
  •  154
    Preparing undergraduates for visual analytics
    IEEE Computer Graphics and Applications 35 (2): 16-20. 2015.
    Visual analytics (VA) combines the strengths of human and machine intelligence to enable the discovery of interesting patterns in challenging datasets. Historically, most attention has been given to developing the machine component—for example, machine learning or the human-computer interface. However, it is also essential to develop the abilities of the analysts themselves, especially at the beginning of their careers. For the past several years, we at the University of British Columbia (UBC)—w…Read more
  •  652
    Competition for consciousness among visual events: The psychophysics of reentrant visual processes
    with Vincent Di Lollo and James T. Enns
    Journal Of Experimental Psychology-General 129 (4): 481-507. 2000.
    Advances in neuroscience implicate reentrant signaling as the predominant form of communication between brain areas. This principle was used in a series of masking experiments that defy explanation by feed-forward theories. The masking occurs when a brief display of target plus mask is continued with the mask alone. Two masking processes were found: an early process affected by physical factors such as adapting luminance and a later process affected by attentional factors such as set size. This …Read more
  •  286
    This article discusses several key issues concerning the study of attention and its relation to visual perception, with an emphasis on behavioral and experiential aspects. It begins with an overview of several classical works carried out in the latter half of the 20th century, such as the development of early filter and spotlight models of attention. This is followed by a survey of subsequent research that extended or modified these results in significant ways. It covers current work on various …Read more
  •  228
    Group-level differences in visual search asymmetry
    with Emily S. Cramer and Michelle J. Dusko
    Attention Perception and Psychophysics 78 1585-1602. 2016.
    East Asians and Westerners differ in various aspects of perception and cognition. For example, visual memory for East Asians is believed to be more influenced by the contextual aspects of a scene than is the case for Westerners (Masuda & Nisbett, 2001). There are also differences in visual search: for Westerners, search for a long line among short is faster than for short among long, whereas this difference does not appear to hold for East Asians (Ueda et al., submitted). However, it is unclear …Read more
  •  75
    Cognitive principles for information management: The principles of mnemonic associative knowledge (P-MAK)
    with Michael Huggett and Holger Hoos
    Minds and Machines 17 (4): 445-485. 2007.
    Information management systems improve the retention of information in large collections. As such they act as memory prostheses, implying an ideal basis in human memory models. Since humans process information by association, and situate it in the context of space and time, systems should maximize their effectiveness by mimicking these functions. Since human attentional capacity is limited, systems should scaffold cognitive efforts in a comprehensible manner. We propose the Principles of Mnemoni…Read more
  •  500
    Change Blindness and Inattentional Blindness
    In William Banks (ed.), Encyclopedia of Consciousness, vol 1, Elsevier. pp. 47-59. 2009.
    As observers, we generally have a strong impression of seeing everything in front of us at any moment. But compelling as it is, this impression is false – there are severe limits to what we can consciously experience in everyday life. Much of the evidence for this claim has come from two phenomena: change blindness (CB) and inattentional blindness (IB). CB refers to the failure of an observer to visually experience changes that are easily seen once noticed. This can happen even if the changes ar…Read more
  •  298
    A Function-Centered Taxonomy of Visual Attention
    In Paul Coates & Sam Coleman (eds.), Phenomenal Qualities: Sense, Perception, and Consciousness, Oxford University Press. pp. 347-375. 2015.
    It is suggested that the relationship between visual attention and conscious visual experience can be simplified by distinguishing different aspects of both visual attention and visual experience. A set of principles is first proposed for any possible taxonomy of the processes involved in visual attention. A particular taxonomy is then put forward that describes five such processes, each with a distinct function and characteristic mode of operation. Based on these, three separate kinds—or possib…Read more
  •  188
    Early completion of occluded objects
    with James T. Enns
    Vision Research 38 2489-2505. 1998.
    We show that early vision can use monocular cues to rapidly complete partially-occluded objects. Visual search for easily detected fragments becomes difficult when the completed shape is similar to others in the display; conversely, search for fragments that are difficult to detect becomes easy when the completed shape is distinctive. Results indicate that completion occurs via the occlusion-triggered removal of occlusion edges and linking of associated regions. We fail to find evidence for a vi…Read more
  •  494
    Picture changes during blinks: Looking without seeing and seeing without looking
    with J. Kevin O'Regan, H. Deubel, and James J. Clark
    Visual Cognition 7 191-211. 2000.
    Observers inspected normal, high quality color displays of everyday visual scenes while their eye movements were recorded. A large display change occurred each time an eye blink occurred. Display changes could either involve "Central Interest" or "Marginal Interest" locations, as determined from descriptions obtained from independent judges in a prior pilot experiment. Visual salience, as determined by luminance, color, and position of the Central and Marginal interest changes were equalized. Th…Read more
  •  234
    The nature of correlation perception in scatterplots
    Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 24 (3): 776-797. 2017.
    For scatterplots with gaussian distributions of dots, the perception of Pearson correlation r can be described by two simple laws: a linear one for discrimination, and a logarithmic one for perceived magnitude (Rensink & Baldridge, 2010). The underlying perceptual mechanisms, however, remain poorly understood. To cast light on these, four different distributions of datapoints were examined. The first had 100 points with equal variance in both dimensions. Consistent with earlier results, just not…Read more
  •  651
    To have seen or not to have seen: A Look at Rensink, O’Regan, and Clark (1997)
    Perspectives on Psychological Science 13 (2). 2018.
    Rensink, O’Regan, and Clark (1997) drew attention to the phenomenon of change blindness, in which even large changes can be difficult to notice if made during the appearance of motion transients elsewhere in the image. This article provides a sketch of the events that inspired that article as well as its subsequent impact on psychological science and on society at large.
  •  269
    On the Prospects for a Science of Visualization
    In Handbook of Human-Centric Visualization, Springer. pp. 147-175. 2014.
    This paper explores the extent to which a scientific framework for visualization might be possible. It presents several potential parts of a framework, illustrated by application to the visualization of correlation in scatterplots. The first is an extended-vision thesis, which posits that a viewer and visualization system can be usefully considered as a single system that perceives structure in a dataset, much like "basic" vision perceives structure in the world. This characterization is then us…Read more
  •  505
    Our visual experience of the world is one of diverse objects and events, each with particular colors, shapes, and motions. This experience is so coherent, so immediate, and so effortless that it seems to result from a single system that lets us experience everything in our field of view. But however appealing, this belief is mistaken: there are severe limits on what can be visually experienced. For example, in a display for air-traffic control it is important to track all moving items. For a sin…Read more
  •  240
    A framework for using magic to study the mind
    with Gustav Kuhn
    Frontiers in Psychology 5 (1508): 1-14. 2015.
    Over the centuries, magicians have developed extensive knowledge about the manipulation of the human mind—knowledge that has been largely ignored by psychology. It has recently been argued that this knowledge could help improve our understanding of human cognition and consciousness. But how might this be done? And how much could it ultimately contribute to the exploration of the human mind? We propose here a framework outlining how knowledge about magic can be used to help us understand the huma…Read more
  •  262
    Visualization as a stimulus domain for vision science
    Journal of Vision 21 (3). 2021.
    Traditionally, vision science and information/data visualization have interacted by using knowledge of human vision to help design effective displays. It is argued here, however, that this interaction can also go in the opposite direction: the investigation of successful visualizations can lead to the discovery of interesting new issues and phenomena in visual perception. Various studies are reviewed showing how this has been done for two areas of visualization, namely, graphical representations…Read more
  •  55
    Cultural differences in visual search for geometric figures
    with Yoshiyuki Ueda, Lei Chen, Jonathon Kopecky, Emily S. Cramer, David E. Meyer, Shinobu Kitayama, and Jun Saiki
    Cognitive Science 42 (1): 286-310. 2018.
    While some studies suggest cultural differences in visual processing, others do not, possibly because the complexity of their tasks draws upon high-level factors that could obscure such effects. To control for this, we examined cultural differences in visual search for geometric figures, a relatively simple task for which the underlying mechanisms are reasonably well known. We replicated earlier results showing that North Americans had a reliable search asymmetry for line length: Search for long…Read more
  •  444
    Change blindness, representations, and consciousness: Reply to Noe
    Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9 (5): 219. 2005.
    Our recent opinion article [1] examined what change blindness can and cannot tell us about visual representations. Among other things, we argued that change blindness can tell us a lot about how visual representations can be used, but little about their extent. We and others found the ‘sparse representations’ view appealing (and still do), and initially made the overly strong claim that change blindness supports the conclusion of sparse representations [2,3]. We wrote our article because change…Read more
  •  327
    Visual sensing without seeing
    Psychological Science 15 27-32. 2004.
    It has often been assumed that when we use vision to become aware of an object or event in our surroundings, this must be accompanied by a corresponding visual experience (i.e., seeing). The studies reported here show that this assumption is incorrect. When observers view a sequence of displays alternating between an image of a scene and the same image changed in some way, they often feel (or sense) the change even though they have no visual experience of it. The subjective difference between se…Read more
  •  2756
    To see or not to see: The need for attention to perceive changes in scenes
    with J. Kevin O'Regan and James J. Clark
    Psychological Science 8 368-373. 1997.
    When looking at a scene, observers feel that they see its entire structure in great detail and can immediately notice any changes in it. However, when brief blank fields are placed between alternating displays of an original and a modified scene, a striking failure of perception is induced: identification of changes becomes extremely difficult, even when changes are large and made repeatedly. Identification is much faster when a verbal cue is provided, showing that poor visibility is not the c…Read more
  •  503
    When good observers go bad: Change blindness, inattentional blindness, and visual experience
    PSYCHE: An Interdisciplinary Journal of Research On Consciousness 6 (9). 2000.
    Several studies (e.g., Becklen & Cervone, 1983; Mack & Rock, 1998; Neisser & Becklen, 1975) have found that observers attending to a particular object or event often fail to report the presence of unexpected items. This has been interpreted as inattentional blindness (IB), a failure to see unattended items (Mack & Rock, 1998). Meanwhile, other studies (e.g., Pashler, 1988; Phillips, 1974; Rensink et al., 1997; Simons, 1996) have found that observers often fail to report the presence of large cha…Read more
  •  140
    The world, the brain, and the speed of sight
    In David Knill & Whitman Richards (eds.), Perception as Bayesian Inference, Cambridge University Press. pp. 495-498. 1996.
    Adelson & Pentland (Chapter 11) use an engaging metaphor to illustrate their position on scene analysis: interpretations are produced by a workshop that employs a set of specialists, each concerned with a single aspect of the scene. The authors argue that it is too expensive to have a supervisor co-ordinate the specialists and that it is too expensive to let them operate independently. They then show that a careful sequencing of the specialists leads to solutions of minimum cost, at least for th…Read more
  •  1727
    Change blindness: Past, present, and future (review)
    Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9 (1): 16-20. 2005.
    Change blindness is the striking failure to see large changes that normally would be noticed easily. Over the past decade this phenomenon has greatly contributed to our understanding of attention, perception, and even consciousness. The surprising extent of change blindness explains its broad appeal, but its counterintuitive nature has also engendered confusions about the kinds of inferences that legitimately follow from it. Here we discuss the legitimate and the erroneous inferences that have b…Read more
  •  222
    Visual Attention
    In L. Nagel (ed.), Encyclopedia of Cognitive Science, Macmillan. 2002.
    Selective access and integration underlie much of our visual experience. This article describes several of the experimental techniques used to investigate these processes, and some of the major results achieved in our understanding of their operation.
  •  187
    The Rapid Recovery of Three-Dimensional Structure from Line Drawings
    Dissertation, University of British Columbia. 1992.
    A computational theory is developed that explains how line drawings of polyhedral objects can be interpreted rapidly and in parallel at early levels of human vision. The key idea is that a time-limited process can correctly recover much of the three-dimensional structure of these objects when split into concurrent streams, each concerned with a single aspect of scene structure.
  •  697
    Towards a science of magic
    with Gustav Kuhn and Alym A. Amlani
    Trends in Cognitive Sciences 12 (9): 349-354. 2008.
    It is argued here that cognitive science currently neglects an important source of insight into the human mind: the effects created by magicians. Over the centuries, magicians have learned how to perform acts that are perceived as defying the laws of nature, and that induce a strong sense of wonder. This article argues that the time has come to examine the scientific bases behind such phenomena, and to create a science of magic linked to relevant areas of cognitive science. Concrete examples ar…Read more