• PhilPapers
  • PhilPeople
  • PhilArchive
  • PhilEvents
  • PhilJobs
  • Sign in
PhilPeople
 
  • Sign in
  • News Feed
  • Find Philosophers
  • Departments
  • Radar
  • Help
 
profile-cover
Drag to reposition
profile picture

Gabriel Finkelstein

University of Colorado Denver
  •  Home
  •  Publications
    32
    • Most Recent
    • Most Downloaded
    • Topics
  •  News and Updates
    10

 More details
  • University of Colorado Denver
    History
    Associate Professor
Princeton University
Department of History
PhD, 1996
Email (login required)
CV
Homepage
0000-0003-2447-0448
Areas of Specialization
History of Neuroscience
History of Biology
History of Science, Misc
19th Century German Philosophy, Misc
Philosophy of History
Areas of Interest
19th Century Philosophy
General Philosophy of Science
History of Western Philosophy
Philosophy of Mind
  • All publications (32)
  •  2
    Paris or Berlin? Claude Bernard’s rivalry with Emil du Bois-Reymond
    History and Philosophy of the Life Sciences 45 (2): 1-21. 2023.
    Claude Bernard (1813–1878) and Emil du Bois-Reymond (1818–1896) rank as two of the most influential scientists of the nineteenth century. Renowned for their experiments, lectures, and writing, Bernard and du Bois-Reymond earned great prestige as professors of physiology in a time when Paris and Berlin reigned as capitals of science. Yet even though they were equals in every way, du Bois-Reymond’s reputation has fallen far more than Bernard’s. This essay compares aspects of the two men’s attitude…Read more
    Claude Bernard (1813–1878) and Emil du Bois-Reymond (1818–1896) rank as two of the most influential scientists of the nineteenth century. Renowned for their experiments, lectures, and writing, Bernard and du Bois-Reymond earned great prestige as professors of physiology in a time when Paris and Berlin reigned as capitals of science. Yet even though they were equals in every way, du Bois-Reymond’s reputation has fallen far more than Bernard’s. This essay compares aspects of the two men’s attitudes to philosophy, history, and biology in an attempt to explain why Bernard remains the better known. The answer lies less in the value of du Bois-Reymond’s contributions than in the way that science is remembered in France and Germany.
    Philosophy of Biology
  •  1
    M. Norton Wise, Aesthetics, Industry, and Science: Hermann von Helmholtz and the Berlin Physical Society. Chicago and London: University of Chicago Press. 2018. Xxi + 405 pp. $45.00 (review)
    German History 38 (1). 2020.
    Aesthetics
  •  2
    M. Norton Wise, Aesthetics, Industry, and Science: Hermann von Helmholtz and the Berlin Physical Society, Chicago; London: University of Chicago Press, 2018 (review)
    German History 38 141-142. 2020.
    Book review
    Aesthetics
  •  50
    Stephen Gaukroger, Civilization and the culture of science: Science and the shaping of modernity, 1795–1935. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press, 2020, 544 pp., ISBN: 978‐0‐19‐884907‐0, $50.00 (review)
    European Journal of Philosophy 29 (1): 256-259. 2021.
    European Journal of Philosophy, Volume 29, Issue 1, Page 256-259, March 2021.
  • Laura Meneghello, Jacob Moleschott—A Transnational Biography: Science, Politics, and Popularization in Nineteenth-Century Europe, Bielefeld: Transcript-Verlag, 2018. Isis 109, no. 4 (December 2018): 874–875 (review)
    Isis 109. 2018.
    History of Western PhilosophyHistory of Science, MiscSociology of Science
  •  377
    Haeckel and du Bois-Reymond: Rival German Darwinists
    Theory in Biosciences 1-8. 2019.
    Ernst Haeckel and Emil du Bois-Reymond were the most prominent champions of Darwin in Germany. This essay compares their contributions to popularizing the theory of evolution, drawing special attention to the neglected figure of du Bois-Reymond as a spokesman for a world devoid of natural purpose. It suggests that the historiography of the German reception of Darwin’s theory needs to be reassessed in the light of du Bois-Reymond’s Lucretian outlook.
    Darwinism
  • Emil du Bois-Reymond: The making of a liberal German scientist (1818-1851)
    Dissertation, . 1996.
    History of Neuroscience
  • Marriage and Science in Nineteenth Century Berlin: Emil du Bois-Reymond’s Correspondence with Jeannette Claude
    In B. P. Wanrooij (ed.), La Mediazione matrimoniale: Il Terzo (in)comodo in Europa fra Otto e Novecento, . pp. 195-219. 2004.
    Other Academic Areas, MiscArts and Humanities
  • Headless in Kashgar
    Endeavour 23 (1): 5-9. 1999.
    In 1854 the British East India Company, acting in co-operation with the Prussian Crown, commissioned Hermann, Adolph and Robert Schlagintweit to undertake a scientific expedition to India and High Asia. Despite the mission's outstanding achievements, all the brothers ended forgotten and miserable. This article will discuss (1) how three sons of a Munich eye surgeon attracted and lost so much high-level attention, and (2) what the Schlagintweits' successes and failures tell us about British and G…Read more
    In 1854 the British East India Company, acting in co-operation with the Prussian Crown, commissioned Hermann, Adolph and Robert Schlagintweit to undertake a scientific expedition to India and High Asia. Despite the mission's outstanding achievements, all the brothers ended forgotten and miserable. This article will discuss (1) how three sons of a Munich eye surgeon attracted and lost so much high-level attention, and (2) what the Schlagintweits' successes and failures tell us about British and German science in the middle of the 19th century.
    History of Science, Misc
  •  160
    Autorité rhétorique: Claude Bernard et Émile du Bois-Reymond
    In Jean-Gäel Barbara & Pierre Corvol (eds.), Les élèves de Claude Bernard: Les nouvelles disciplines bernardiennes au tournant du XXe siècle, . pp. 173-192. 2012.
    Professeur Finkelstein avait posée la question, pourquoi, bien que leurs réalisations scientifiques et leur scientifique approche soient similaires, Bernard était beaucoup plus connu dans son pays, France, et à son époque, que Bois-Reymond en Allemagne? Une question similaire a été posée au sujet du pourquoi Darwin est connu pour la théorie de l'évolution, tandis que Wallace a été remis en arrière-fond dans leur temps et dans l'histoire. Selon Finkelstein, la cause de la differences entre Bois-R…Read more
    Professeur Finkelstein avait posée la question, pourquoi, bien que leurs réalisations scientifiques et leur scientifique approche soient similaires, Bernard était beaucoup plus connu dans son pays, France, et à son époque, que Bois-Reymond en Allemagne? Une question similaire a été posée au sujet du pourquoi Darwin est connu pour la théorie de l'évolution, tandis que Wallace a été remis en arrière-fond dans leur temps et dans l'histoire. Selon Finkelstein, la cause de la differences entre Bois-Reymond et Bernard, peut être trouvée dans la culture et la place de la science dans l'œil public en France contre les memes choses en Allemagne. C'est-à-dire, la rhétorique de la science a été plus respectée en France qu'en Allemagne, ce que Finkelstein a soutenu avec persuasion et éloquence.
    History of Science, MiscHistory of Biology
  •  582
    Emil du Bois-Reymond on "The Seat of the Soul"
    Journal of the History of the Neurosciences 23 (1): 45-55. 2014.
    The German pioneer of electrophysiology, Emil du Bois-Reymond (1818–1896), is generally assumed to have remained silent on the subject of the brain. However, the archive of his papers in Berlin contains manuscript notes to a lecture on “The Seat of the Soul” that he delivered to popular audiences in 1884 and 1885. These notes demonstrate that cerebral localization and brain function in general had been concerns of his for quite some time, and that he did not shy away from these subjects.
    Consciousness and Neuroscience, Foundational IssuesHistory of Neuroscience
  • The Mountains and the Sea: Travel as Discovery in the Lives of Emil du Bois-Reymond and Ernst Haeckel.
    Chronica Mundi 9 (1): 182-192. 2015.
    History of BiologyOther Academic Areas
  •  135
    Gustav Magnus und sein Haus: Im Auftrag der Deutschen Physikalischen Gesellschaft, ed. Dieter Hoffmann, Stuttgart: Verlag für Geschichte der Naturwissenschaften und der Technik, 1995 (review)
    Technology and Culture 39 (3): 568-569. 1998.
    History of Physics
  •  147
    Michel Meulders, Helmholtz, des lumières aux neurosciences, Paris: Editions Odile Jacob, 2001 (review)
    Journal of the History of the Neurosciences 11 (3): 317-319. 2002.
    History of NeuroscienceHistory of PsychologyHistory of Physics
  •  129
    Daniel P. Todes, Pavlov’s Physiology Factory: Experiment, Interpretation, Laboratory Enterprise, Baltimore: John Hopkins, 2002 (review)
    Journal of the History of the Neurosciences 14 (1): 70-71. 2005.
    History of Psychology
  •  72
    Russell Stannard, The End of Discovery: Are We Approaching the Boundaries of the Knowable? Oxford; New York: Oxford University, 2010 (review)
    Journal of Scientific Exploration 8 (4): 838. 2011.
    Science, Logic, and Mathematics
  •  289
    The Ascent of Man? Emil du Bois-Reymond's Reflections on Scientific Progress
    Endeavour 24 (3): 129-132. 2000.
    Triumphalist histories of science are nothing new but were, in fact, a staple of the 19th century. This article considers one of the more famous works in the genre and argues that it was motivated by doubt more than by faith.
    History of Western Philosophy
  •  315
    Emil du Bois-Reymond vs Ludimar Hermann
    Comptes Rendus Biologies 329 (5-6): 340-347. 2006.
    This essay recounts a controversy between a pioneer electrophysiologist, Emil du Bois-Reymond (1818–1896), and his student, Ludimar Hermann (1838–1914). Du Bois-Reymond proposed a molecular explanation for the slight electrical currents that he detected in frog muscles and nerves. Hermann argued that du Bois-Reymond's ‘resting currents’ were an artifact of injury to living tissue. He contested du Bois-Reymond's molecular model, explaining his teacher's observations as electricity produced by che…Read more
    This essay recounts a controversy between a pioneer electrophysiologist, Emil du Bois-Reymond (1818–1896), and his student, Ludimar Hermann (1838–1914). Du Bois-Reymond proposed a molecular explanation for the slight electrical currents that he detected in frog muscles and nerves. Hermann argued that du Bois-Reymond's ‘resting currents’ were an artifact of injury to living tissue. He contested du Bois-Reymond's molecular model, explaining his teacher's observations as electricity produced by chemical decomposition. History has painted Hermann as the wronged party in this dispute. I seek to set the record straight.
    Science, Logic, and MathematicsHistory of Western Philosophy
  •  213
    Matteucci and du Bois-Reymond: A Bitter Rivalry
    Archives Italiennes de Biologie 149 (4): 29-37. 2011.
    This essay considers a long-standing controversy between two nineteenth century pioneers in electrophysiology: the German scientist Emil du Bois-Reymond (1818-1896), and his Italian rival Carlo Matteucci (1811-1868). Historians have generally described their disagreement in du Bois-Reymond’s terms: the product of a contrast in scientific outlook. While not discounting this interpretation, I want to suggest that the controversy was driven as much by the rivals’ similarity as it was by their diffe…Read more
    This essay considers a long-standing controversy between two nineteenth century pioneers in electrophysiology: the German scientist Emil du Bois-Reymond (1818-1896), and his Italian rival Carlo Matteucci (1811-1868). Historians have generally described their disagreement in du Bois-Reymond’s terms: the product of a contrast in scientific outlook. While not discounting this interpretation, I want to suggest that the controversy was driven as much by the rivals’ similarity as it was by their difference.
    History of Biology
  •  143
    Why Darwin was English
    Endeavour 24 (2): 76-78. 2000.
    A ‘late developer’ argument, common to Psychology and Economic History, can be used to explain cultural innovation. It argues that the 19th century theory of natural selection arose in England and not Germany because of – and not in spite of – England’s scientific backwardness. Measured in terms of institutions, communities, and ideas, the relative retardation of English science was precisely what enabled it to adopt German advances in novel ways.
    19th Century Philosophy, MiscellaneousRace as Socially Constructed
  •  137
    Does God Play Dice? Roger Penrose, Quantum Consciousness, and the Debate Over the Limits of Science
    A talk delivered at the conference “Science and Religion: The Religious Beliefs and Practices of Scientists—20th Century,” Institut für Wissenschaftsgeschichte, Georg-August-Universität, Göttingen, 28 May 2002
    History of Western Philosophy
  •  141
    Response to Richards
    In Kristin Gjesdal (ed.), Debates in Nineteenth-Century European Philosophy: Essential Readings and Contemporary Responses, . pp. 226-230. 2016.
    Emil du Bois-Reymond (1818-1896) complicates the historiography of the reception of Darwinism. His presentation of the theory was anti-teleological, a fact that refutes the claim that German Darwinists were Romantic.
    19th Century Philosophy, MiscellaneousDarwinism
  •  136
    Romanticism, Race, and Recapitulation
    Science 294 (5549): 2101-2102. 2001.
    Why race persists as an idea despite its scientific inutility.
    History of Western Philosophy
  •  187
    Mechanical Neuroscience: Emil du Bois-Reymond’s Innovations in Theory and Practice
    Frontiers 9 (130): 1-4. 2015.
    Summary of the major innovations of Emil du Bois-Reymond (1818-1896)
    Science and Religion19th Century French Philosophy, Misc
  •  15
    John A. McCarthy; Stephanie M. Hilger; Heather I. Sullivan; Nicholas Saul, The Early History of Embodied Cognition, 1740–1920: The Lebenskraft-Debate and Radical Reality in German Science, Music, and Literature. 357 pp., bibl. Leiden: Brill, 2016. €99 (review)
    Isis 108 (1): 200-201. 2017.
    Book review of contributions from scholars of 19th-century German
  •  13
    Peter Meusburger; Thomas Schuch, eds. Wissenschaftsatlas of Heidelberg University: Spatio-Temporal Relations of Academic Knowledge Production. 391 pp., illus., apps., bibl. Knittlingen: Verlag Bibliotheca Palatina, 2012. €129 (review)
    Isis 105 (2): 418-419. 2014.
    HistoryHistory of Science
  •  3
    Karl Clausberg. Zwischen den Sternen: Lichtbildarchive: Was Einstein und Uexküll, Benjamin und das Kino der Astronomie des 19. Jahrhunderts verdanken. x + 270 pp., illus., figs., apps., bibl., indexes. Berlin: Akademie Verlag, 2006. €49.80 (review)
    Isis 100 (1): 171-171. 2009.
    History of Science
  •  287
    “Conquerors of The Künlün”? The Schlagintweit Mission to High Asia, 1854–57
    History of Science 38 (2): 179-218. 2000.
    Backstory of "The Man Who Would Be King." A meditation on the limits of scientific and historical representation.
    History
  •  37
    M. du Bois-Reymond Goes To Paris
    British Journal for the History of Science 36 (3): 261-300. 2003.
    This article examines the science of electrophysiology developed by Emil du Bois-Reymond in Berlin in the 1840s. In it I recount his major findings, the most significant being his proof of the electrical nature of nerve signals. Du Bois-Reymond also went on to detect this same ‘negative variation’, or action current, in live human subjects. In 1850 he travelled to Paris to defend this startling claim. The essay concludes with a discussion of why his demonstration failed to convince his hosts at …Read more
    This article examines the science of electrophysiology developed by Emil du Bois-Reymond in Berlin in the 1840s. In it I recount his major findings, the most significant being his proof of the electrical nature of nerve signals. Du Bois-Reymond also went on to detect this same ‘negative variation’, or action current, in live human subjects. In 1850 he travelled to Paris to defend this startling claim. The essay concludes with a discussion of why his demonstration failed to convince his hosts at the French Academy of Sciences.
    History of Neuroscience
  •  15
    Emil du Bois-Reymond's Reflections on Consciousness
    In Chris Smith Harry Whitaker (ed.), Brain, Mind and Consciousness in the History of Neuroscience, Springer. pp. 163-184. 2014.
    The late 19th-century Ignorabimus controversy over the limits of scientific knowledge has often been characterized as proclaiming the end of intellectual progress, and by implication, as plunging Germany into a crisis of pessimism from which Liberalism never recovered. My research supports the opposite interpretation. The initiator of the Ignorabimus controversy, Emil du Bois-Reymond, was a physiologist who worked his whole life against the forces of obscurantism, whether they came from the Cath…Read more
    The late 19th-century Ignorabimus controversy over the limits of scientific knowledge has often been characterized as proclaiming the end of intellectual progress, and by implication, as plunging Germany into a crisis of pessimism from which Liberalism never recovered. My research supports the opposite interpretation. The initiator of the Ignorabimus controversy, Emil du Bois-Reymond, was a physiologist who worked his whole life against the forces of obscurantism, whether they came from the Catholic and Conservative Right or the scientistic and millenarian Left. Du Bois-Reymond’s doubt that scientists would ever elucidate consciousness must therefore be seen as an endorsement, and not a rejection, of his faith in reason.
    Consciousness and Neuroscience, Foundational Issues
  • Prev.
  • 1
  • 2
  • Next
PhilPeople logo

On this site

  • Find a philosopher
  • Find a department
  • The Radar
  • Index of professional philosophers
  • Index of departments
  • Help
  • Acknowledgments
  • Careers
  • Contact us
  • Terms and conditions

Brought to you by

  • The PhilPapers Foundation
  • The American Philosophical Association
  • Centre for Digital Philosophy, Western University
PhilPeople is currently in Beta Sponsored by the PhilPapers Foundation and the American Philosophical Association
Feedback